9.1 - Log-transforming Only the Predictor for SLR

In this section, we learn how to build and use a simple linear regression model by transforming the predictor x values. This might be the first thing that you try if you find a lack of linear trend in your data. That is, transforming the x values is appropriate when non-linearity is the only problem — the independence, normality, and equal variance conditions are met. Note, though, that it may be necessary to correct the non-linearity before you can assess the normality and equal variance assumptions. Also, while some assumptions may appear to hold prior to applying a transformation, they may no longer hold once a transformation is applied. In other words, using transformations is part of an iterative process where all the linear regression assumptions are re-checked after each iteration.

Keep in mind that although we're focussing on a simple linear regression model here, the essential ideas apply more generally to multiple linear regression models too. We can consider transforming any of the predictors by examining scatterplots of the residuals versus each predictor in turn.

Example 9-1: Word Recall Section

Building the model

The easiest way to learn about data transformations is by example. Let's consider the data from a memory retention experiment in which 13 subjects were asked to memorize a list of disconnected items. The subjects were then asked to recall the items at various times up to a week later. The proportion of items (y = prop) correctly recalled at various times (x = time, in minutes) since the list was memorized were recorded (Word Recall data) and plotted. Recognizing that there is no good reason that the error terms would not be independent, let's evaluate the remaining three conditions — linearity, normality, and equal variances — of the model.

The resulting fitted line plot suggests that the proportion of recalled items (y) is not linearly related to time (x):

prop vs time plot

The residuals vs. fits plot also suggests that the relationship is not linear:

residuals vs fitted values plot

Because the lack of linearity dominates the plot, we cannot use the plot to evaluate whether or not the error variances are equal. We have to fix the non-linearity problem before we can assess the assumption of equal variances.

What about the normal probability plot of the residuals? What does it suggest about the error terms? Can we conclude that they are normally distributed?

normal probability plot

The Ryan-Joiner P-value for this example is large, which suggests that we fail to reject the null hypothesis of normal error terms. There is not enough evidence to conclude that the error terms are not normal.

In summary, we have a data set in which non-linearity is the only major problem. This situation screams out for transforming only the predictor's values. Before we do so, let's take an aside and discuss the "logarithmic transformation," since it is the most common and most useful data transformation available.


The Logarithmic Transformation

The default logarithmic transformation merely involves taking the natural logarithm — denoted \(ln\) or \(log_e\) or simply \(log\) — of each data value. One could consider taking a different kind of logarithm, such as log base 10 or log base 2. However, the natural logarithm — which can be thought of as log base e where e is the constant \(2.718282 \dots\) — is the most common logarithmic scale used in scientific work.

The general characteristics of the natural logarithmic function are:
  • The natural logarithm of x is the power of \(e = 2.718282 \dots\) that you have to take in order to get x. This can be stated notationally as \(ln\left(e^{x}\right) = x\). For example, the natural logarithm of 5 is the power to which you have to raise e = 2.718282... to get 5. Since \(2.718282^{1.60944}\) is approximately 5, we say that the natural logarithm of 5 is 1.60944. Notationally, we say \(ln\left(5\right) = 1.60944\).
  • The natural logarithm of e is equal to one, that is, \(ln\left(e\right) = 1\).
  • The natural logarithm of one is equal to zero, that is, \(ln\left(1\right) = 0\).

The plot of the natural logarithm function:

log function

suggests that the effects of taking the natural logarithmic transformation are:

  • Small values that are close together are spread further out.
  • Large values that are spread out are brought closer together.

Back to the example. Let's use the natural logarithm to transform the x values in the memory retention experiment data. Since x = time is the predictor, all we need to do is take the natural logarithm of each time value appearing in the data set. In doing so, we create a newly transformed predictor called \(lntime \colon\)

time prop \(\text{lntime}\)
1 0.84 0.00000
5 0.71 1.60944
15 0.61 2.70805
30 0.56 3.40120
60 0.54 4.09434
120 0.47 4.78749
240 0.45 5.48064
480 0.38 6.17379
720 0.36 6.57925
1440 0.26 7.27240
2880 0.20 7.96555
5760 0.16 8.65869
10080 0.08 9.21831

We take the natural logarithm for each value of time and place the results in their column. That is, we "transform" each predictor time value to a \(\boldsymbol{\ln\left(\text{time}\right)}\) value. For example, \(ln\left(1\right) = 0\), \(\ln\left(5\right) = 1.60944\), and \(\ln\left(15\right) = 2.70805\), and so on.

Now that we've transformed the predictor values, let's see if it helped correct the non-linear trend in the data. We re-fit the model with \(y = prop\) as the response and \(x = lntime\) as the predictor.

The resulting fitted line plot suggests that taking the natural logarithm of the predictor values is helpful.

fitted line plot using ln(time)

Indeed, the \(r^{2}\) value has increased from 57.1% to 99.0%. It tells us that 99% of the variation in the proportion of recalled words (prop) is reduced by taking into account the natural log of time \(\left(lntime\right)\)!

The new residual vs. fits plot shows a significant improvement over the one based on the untransformed data.

residuals vs fitted values plot

You might become concerned about some kind of an up-down cyclical trend in the plot. I caution you again not to over-interpret these plots, especially when the data set is small like this. You really shouldn't expect perfection when you resort to taking data transformations. Sometimes you have to just be content with significant improvements. By the way, the plot also suggests that it is okay to assume that the error variances are equal.

The normal probability plot of the residuals shows that transforming the x values did not affect the normality of the error terms:

normal probability plot

Again the Ryan-Joiner P-value is large, so we fail to reject the null hypothesis of normal error terms. There is not enough evidence to conclude that the error terms are not normal.

What if we had transformed the y values instead? Earlier I said that while some assumptions may appear to hold before applying a transformation, they may no longer hold once a transformation is applied. For example, if the error terms are well-behaved, transforming the y values could change them into badly-behaved error terms. The error terms for the memory retention data before transforming the x values appear to be well-behaved (in the sense that they appear approximately normal). Therefore, we might expect that transforming the y values instead of the x values could cause the error terms to become badly behaved. Let's take a quick look at the memory retention data to see an example of what can happen when we transform the y values when non-linearity is the only problem.

By trial and error, we discover that the power transformation of y that does the best job at correcting the non-linearity is \(y^{-1.25}\). The fitted line plot illustrates that the transformation does indeed straighten out the relationship — although admittedly not as well as the log transformation of the x values.

fitted line plot using prop^-1.26

Note that the \(r^{2}\) value has increased from 57.1% to 96.4%.

The residuals show an improvement concerning non-linearity, although the improvement is not great...

residuals vs fitted values plot

...but now we have non-normal error terms! The Ryan-Joiner P-value is smaller than 0.01, so we reject the null hypothesis of normal error terms. There is sufficient evidence to conclude that the error terms are not normal:

normal probability plot

Again, if the error terms are well-behaved before transformation, transforming the y values can change them into badly-behaved error terms.


Using the Model

Once we've found the best model for our regression data, we can then use the model to answer our research questions of interest. If our model involves transformed predictor (x) values, we may or may not have to make slight modifications to the standard procedures we've already learned.

Let's use our linear regression model for the memory retention data — with \(y = prop\) as the response and \(x = lntime\) as the predictor — to answer four different research questions.

Research Question #1: What is the nature of the association between time since memorization and the effectiveness of recall?

fitted line plot using ln(time)

To answer this research question, we just describe the nature of the relationship. That is, the proportion of correctly recalled words is negatively linearly related to the natural log of the time since the words were memorized. Not surprisingly, as the natural log of time increases, the proportion of recalled words decreases.

Research Question #2: Is there an association between time since memorization and the effectiveness of recall?

In answering this research question, no modification to the standard procedure is necessary. We merely test the null hypothesis \(H_0 \colon \beta_1 = 0 \) using either the F-test or the equivalent t-test:

The regression equation is

\(\widehat{prop}\) = 0.846 - 0.0792 lntime

Predictor Coef SE Coef T P
Constant 0.84642 0.01419 59.63 0.000
lntime -0.079227 0.002416 -32.80 0.000

Model Summary

S R-Sq R-Sq(adj) R-sq(pred)
0.02339 99.0% 98.9%  

Analysis of Variance

Source DF SS MS F P
Regression 1 0.58841 0.58841 1075.70 0.000
Residual Error 1 0.00602 0.00055    
Total 12 0.59443      

As the Minitab output illustrates, the P-value is < 0.001. There is significant evidence at the 0.05 level to conclude that there is a linear association between the proportion of words recalled and the natural log of the time since memorization.

Research Question #3: What proportion of words can we expect a randomly selected person to recall after 1000 minutes?

We just need to calculate a prediction interval — with one slight modification — to answer this research question. Our predictor variable is the natural log of time. Therefore, when we ask Minitab to calculate the prediction interval, we have to make sure that we specify the value of the predictor values in the transformed units, not the original units. The natural log of 1000 minutes is 6.91 log minutes. Asking Minitab to calculate a 95% prediction interval when \(lntime = 6.91\), we obtain:

Values of Predictions for New Observations

New Obs lntime
1 6.91

Prediction Values for New Observations

New Obs Fit SE Fit 95% CI 95% PI
1 0.29896 0.00766 (0.282, 0.316) (0.245, 0.353)

The Minitab output tells us that we can be 95% confident that, after 1000 minutes, a randomly selected person will recall between 24.5% and 35.3% of the words.

Research Question #4: How much does the expected recall change if time increases tenfold?

If you think about it, answering this research question merely involves estimating and interpreting the slope parameter \(\beta_1\). Well, not quite — there is a slight adjustment. In general, a k-fold increase in the predictor x is associated with a:

\(\beta_1 \times ln\left(k\right)\)

change in the mean of the response y.

This derivation that follows might help you understand and therefore remember this formula.

That is, a ten-fold increase in x is associated with a \(\beta_1 \times ln\left(10\right)\) change in the mean of y. And, a two-fold increase in x is associated with a \(\beta_1 \times ln\left(2\right)\) change in the mean of y.

In general, you should only use multiples of k that make sense for the scope of the model. For example, if the x values in your data set range from 2 to 8, it only makes sense to consider k multiples that are 4 or smaller. If the value of x were 2, a ten-fold increase (i.e., k = 10) would take you from 2 up to 2 × 10 = 20, a value outside the scope of the model. In the memory retention data set, the predictor values range from 1 to 10080, so there is no problem with considering a ten-fold increase.

If we are only interested in obtaining a point estimate, we merely take the estimate of the slope parameter \(\left(b_1 = -0.079227\right)\) from the Minitab output:

Predictor Coef SE Coef T P
Constant 0.84642 0.01419 59.63 0.000
lntime -0.079227 0.002416 -32.80 0.000

and multiply it by \(ln\left(10\right)\colon\)

\(b_1 \times ln\left(10\right) = -0.079227 \times ln\left(10\right) = - 0.182\)

We expect the percentage of recalled words to decrease (since the sign is negative) by 18.2% for each ten-fold increase in the time since memorization took place.

Of course, this point estimate is of limited usefulness. How confident can we be that the estimate is close to the true unknown value? Naturally, we should calculate a 95% confidence interval. To do so, we just calculate a 95% confidence interval for \(\beta_1\) as we always have:

\( -0.079227 \pm 2.201 \left( 0.002416 \right) = \left( \boldsymbol{ -0.085, -0.074} \right)\)

and then multiply each endpoint of the interval by \(ln\left(10\right)\colon\)

\(-0.074 \times ln\left(10\right) = - \textbf{0.170} \text{ and } -0.085 \times ln\left(10\right) = - \textbf{0.l95}\)

We can be 95% confident that the percentage of recalled words will decrease between 17.0% and 19.5% for each ten-fold increase in the time since memorization took place.